帳號:guest(54.82.44.149)          離開系統
字體大小: 字級放大   字級縮小   預設字形  

詳目顯示

以作者查詢圖書館館藏以作者查詢臺灣博碩士以作者查詢全國書目勘誤回報
作者(中):廖羿淳
作者(英):Liao, I-Tsun
論文名稱(中):以眼動證據探討自閉症類群障礙幼兒的社會注意力:社會定向、共享式注意力與觀察學習
論文名稱(英):From Dyadic Orientation to Triadic Joint Attention to Observational Learning: An Eye-Tracking Study on Social Attention in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
指導教授(中):姜忠信
口試委員:姜忠信
林慧麗
蔡介立
學位類別:碩士
校院名稱:國立政治大學
系所名稱:心理學系
出版年:2020
畢業學年度:108
語文別:中文
論文頁數:136
中文關鍵詞:自閉症類群障礙症社會注意力社會定向共享式注意力觀察學習眼動追蹤技術症狀嚴重度適應行為
英文關鍵詞:Autism spectrum disordersSocial attentionSocial orientationJoint attentionObservational learningEye-trackingSymptom severityAdaptive function
Doi Url:http://doi.org/10.6814/NCCU202001049
相關次數:
  • 推薦推薦:0
  • 點閱點閱:142
  • 評分評分:系統版面圖檔系統版面圖檔系統版面圖檔系統版面圖檔系統版面圖檔
  • 下載下載:15
  • gshot_favorites title msg收藏:0
研究背景與目的:過去探討自閉症類群障礙症(Autism Spectrum Disorder, ASD)的心理病理機制主要皆集中於認知取向,尤以心智理論(theory of mind)為解釋ASD社交溝通及互動方面缺損的核心。然而,體現心智取向(enactive mind approach)認為,ASD兒童可能在早期缺乏對社會訊息的偏好注意,使其知覺經驗偏離一般發展的軌跡,進而影響後續的社會認知能力與社會適應行為。因此,本研究旨在探討ASD幼兒的視覺社會注意力表現,檢驗社會注意力表現與社會缺損的關聯性,進一步探討不同認知能力ASD幼兒的社會注意力表現是否具異質性,繼而回應社會注意力表現用於區別ASD與典型發展(typically developing, TD)幼兒的有效性。
研究方法:本研究共納入59名2-4歲的ASD幼兒與25名配對心理年齡的TD幼兒(1-2.5歲)。採自由觀看的作業典範,以眼動追蹤技術量測幼兒社會定向、共享式注意力與觀察學習等三類的社會注意力表現。本研究亦涵蓋認知能力、症狀嚴重度與適應行為的評估。
研究結果:(1)社會定向方面,ASD幼兒較TD幼兒更多凝視背景、更少凝視嘴部;反應性共享式注意力方面,ASD幼兒較TD幼兒少正確地跟隨視線,且跟隨的時間較慢、對目標物的第一凝視時間亦較短,而主動性共享式注意力之表現兩組未有顯著差異;觀察學習方面,ASD幼兒較TD幼兒少凝視他人命名後的物品,亦較少凝視他人操作物品時的動作區域。(2)以非語言認知能力將ASD幼兒分為高、低功能兩組,發現ASD幼兒的社會定向表現不因認知能力而有所不同,但凝視眼部與高功能ASD幼兒較少的症狀、較佳的適應有關,而凝視嘴部與低功能ASD幼兒較少的症狀有關;反應性共享式注意力的表現亦不因認知能力而有所不同,且較多正確地跟隨他人視線,皆與兩組ASD幼兒較少的症狀有關,但更長的第一凝視時間,反而與低功能ASD幼兒較多的侷限重複行為有關;主動性共享式注意力以高功能ASD幼兒表現較差,觀察學習則以低功能ASD幼兒表現較差,且觀察學習的表現亦與低功能ASD幼兒較少的症狀、較佳的適應有關。(3)社會定向是區別ASD與TD幼兒最有效的指標,可達70%的敏感度與96%的特異度;合併社會定向、反應性共享式注意力與觀察學習三項指標,可達98%的敏感度與68%的特異度;在邏輯迴歸中控制兩組認知能力的差異,社會定向與反應性共享式注意力的表現仍能預測ASD與TD的分組。
結論:研究結果指出,ASD幼兒的社會定向、共享式注意力與觀察學習之表現均有異常,且各面向的社會注意力之間具有關聯,亦與ASD的症狀與適應有關。整體而言,反映社會注意力可能為ASD的早期心理病理機制,且眼動追蹤方法可能作為標定ASD社會注意力異常的有效篩檢工具。
Background and Purposes: Previously, psychopathological studies of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) has mainly focused on cognitive deficit, especially for “theory of mind”, the core theory of explaining dysfunction of social communication and interaction in individuals with ASD. However, through the point of “enactive mind approach”, children with ASD may lack of preferent attentions about social informations in the early stage, which causes their development of perceptive experiences to be deviant from the typical individuals, later on influencing their social cognitive ability and social adaptive behavior. Therefore, this study aimed to investigate the visual social attention in children with ASD and its association with social deficit. Further, concerning the heterogeneity within ASD, the study examined the social attention of children with ASD in different cognitive level, moreover discussing on applying social attention to discriminate children with ASD from children with typical development.
Methods: 59 children aged 2-4 years with ASD and 25 children with typical development (TD) were recruited to be matched on mental age. The current study utilized free-viewing paradigm, and applied eye-tracking techniques to measure the indexes of social-orienting, joint attention, and observational learning. Additionally, cognitive function, symptom severity, and adaptive function were also assessed.
Results: (1) In social orienting task, children with ASD spent proportionally less time viewing the mouth and more time viewing the background. In responding joint attention (RJA) task, children with ASD tracked less correctly with other’s gaze, and the gaze-following response latency was slower, anticipatory gaze duration to the target object was shorter as well. Results showed no significant difference between groups in initiating joint attention (IJA) task. In observational learning tasks, children with ASD spent proportionally less time viewing the named object and the motion area. (2) The children with ASD has been divided into two groups through nonverbal DQ, high- and low-function respectively. No difference has been showed between two groups in social orienting task. For children with high-functioning ASD (HFASD), it showed that fixation on eyes is correlated with less symptoms and better adaptation, whereas for children with low-functioning ASD (LFASD), fixation on mouth was correlated with less symptoms. No difference has been showed between HFASD and LFASD group in RJA task, and correctly gaze-following was correlated with less symptoms in both groups. In IJA task, children with HFASD initiated joint attention slower than children with LFASD. In observational learning task, children with LFASD performed worse, and fixation on target object was showed to be correlated with their less symptoms and better adaptions. (3) Social-orienting was the most accurate indicator to discriminate ASD from TD, with sensitivity of 70%, and specificity of 96%. Combining social-orienting, RJA, and observational learning, it discriminated ASD from TD with sensitivity of 98%, and specificity of 86%. While controlling DQ, still, social-orienting and RJA could significantly discriminate ASD from TD in the logistic regression.
Conclusions: Findings suggested that children with ASD perform abnormally in social-orienting, joint attention and observational learning. Also, these sides of social attention were intercorrelated, which were also correlated with ASD symptoms and adaption. The results implicated that social attention could be one of the early psychopathological mechanism of ASD, and eye-tracking method could be an effective screening tool in defining social attention abnormality in ASD.
第一章 緒論 1
第二章 文獻探討 4
第一節 社會注意力的意涵與重要性 4
第二節 自閉症類群障礙症在社會注意力上的病理機制 8
第三節 社會注意力的眼動研究典範 13
第四節 自閉症類群障礙症患者的社會注意力研究 14
第五節 社會注意力與症狀嚴重度、社會適應行為之關聯性 25
第六節 研究目的與假設 42
第三章 研究方法 45
第一節 研究對象 45
第二節 研究工具 47
第三節 研究程序 54
第四節 資料分析方法 56
第四章 研究結果 60
第一節 前驅分析 60
第二節 自閉症類群障礙幼兒的社會定向表現 62
第三節 自閉症類群障礙幼兒的共享式注意力表現 66
第四節 自閉症類群障礙幼兒的觀察學習注意力表現 71
第五節 社會定向、共享式注意力與觀察學習之關聯 75
第六節 社會注意力與自閉症類群障礙症的異質性 78
第七節 社會注意力與症狀嚴重度、適應行為之關聯 85
第八節 社會注意力對於ASD之分組預測效果 89
第五章 討論 94
第一節 ASD的社會注意力表現 94
第二節 ASD社會注意力的異質性與症狀嚴重度、適應行為之關聯 102
第三節 社會注意力對於ASD之分組預測效果 105
第四節 研究限制與未來研究方向 107
第五節 研究結論與臨床意義 109
參考文獻 114
附錄 132
朱慶琳、姜忠信、李宗芹(2015)。自閉症幼兒的共享式參與家長訓練方案:舞蹈治療之創造性律動取向初探。中華心理衛生學刊,28(1),69-100。
余勝皓、陳學志、林慧麗(2018)。以眼動儀探討罹患自閉症類群障礙症之兒童對自然情境圖片中社會訊息之凝視型態:ASD 自然情境圖片眼動研究。 特殊教育研究學刊,43(2),65-92。
林姿伶、姜忠信、吳欣治(2016)。自閉症類群兒童綜合性早期療育的療效研究:文獻回顧。中華心理衛生學刊,29(1),1-46。
姜忠信、宋維村(2001)。自閉症兒童相互注意協調能力的回顧與前瞻。載於國立台北師院特教中心(主編),自閉症心理與教育論文集(頁125-143)。台北市:國立台北師院特教中心。
劉耿良、顧維倫、陳學志、楊宗仁、林慧麗(2016)。自閉症障礙兒童/青年之臉孔情緒辨識與眼神追蹤眼動表現的關聯性研究。臨床心理學刊,10(1),1-16。
賴孟泉、高淑芬(2010)。自閉症類群。臺灣醫學,14(4),401-411。
鄭昭明(2006)。認知心理學。台北市:桂冠圖書公司。
盧台華、陳心怡(2009)。適應行為評量系統第二版(ABAS–II)中文-幼兒版。台北:中國行為科學社。
Adolphs, R. (2006). How do we know the minds of others? Domain-specificity, simulation, and enactive social cognition. Brain research, 1079(1), 25-35.
Adolphs, R. (2010). Conceptual challenges and directions for social neuroscience. Neuron, 65(6), 752-767.
Aldaqre, I., Paulus, M., & Sodian, B. (2015). Referential gaze and word learning in adults with autism. Autism, 19(8), 944-955.
Alvares, G. A., Chen, N. T., Notebaert, L., Granich, J., Mitchell, C., & Whitehouse, A. J. (2019). Brief social attention bias modification for children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 12(3), 527-535.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-Fifth edition (5th ed.). Washington DC: Author.

Ames, C., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2010). A review of methods in the study of attention in autism. Developmental Review, 30(1), 52-73.
Åsberg Johnels, J., Gillberg, C., Falck-Ytter, T., & Miniscalco, C. (2014). Face-viewing patterns in young children with autism spectrum disorders: speaking up for the role of language comprehension. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(6), 2246-2252.
Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In Moore, C., Dunham, P. J., & Dunham, P. (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131-158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Barenholtz, E., Mavica, L., & Lewkowicz, D. J. (2016). Language familiarity modulates relative attention to the eyes and mouth of a talker. Cognition, 147, 100-105.
Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Current opinion in neurobiology, 23(3), 361-372.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT press.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. In Baron-Cohen, S. (Ed.), The maladapted mind: Classic readings in evolutionary psychopathology (pp. 207-239). Psychology press.
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Moriarty, J., Schmitz, B., Costa, D., & Ell, P. (1994). Recognition of mental terms state terms: Clinical findings in children with autism and a functional neuroimaging study of normal adults. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 640-649.
Bedford, R., Elsabbagh, M., Gliga, T., Pickles, A., Senju, A., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2012). Precursors to social and communication difficulties in infants at-risk for autism: gaze following and attentional engagement. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 42(10), 2208-2218.
Bidet-Ildei, C., Kitromilides, E., Orliaguet, J. P., Pavlova, M., & Gentaz, E. (2013). Preference for point-light human biological motion in newborns: contribution of translational displacement. Developmental psychology, 50(1), 113.
Billeci, L., Narzisi, A., Campatelli, G., Crifaci, G., Calderoni, S., Gagliano, A., ... & Raso, R. (2016). Disentangling the initiation from the response in joint attention: an eye-tracking study in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Translational psychiatry, 6(5), e808.
Birmingham, E., & Kingstone, A. (2009). Human social attention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156(1), 118-140.
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Developmental psychology, 38(6), 958.
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental science, 8(6), 535-543.
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of child language, 35(1), 207-220.
Burnside, K., Wright, K., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2018). Social orienting predicts implicit false belief understanding in preschoolers. Journal of experimental child psychology, 175, 67-79.
Butterworth, G., & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British journal of developmental psychology, 9(1), 55-72.
Campbell, D. J., Shic, F., Macari, S., & Chawarska, K. (2014). Gaze response to dyadic bids at 2 years related to outcomes at 3 years in autism spectrum disorders: a subtyping analysis. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 44(2), 431-442.
Carette, R., Elbattah, M., Cilia, F., Dequen, G., Guérin, J. L., & Bosche, J. (2019). Learning to Predict Autism Spectrum Disorder based on the Visual Patterns of Eye-tracking Scanpaths. Proceedings of the 12th International Joint Conference on Biomedical Engineering Systems and Technologies (BIOSTEC 2019), 103-112.
Carlin, J. D., Rowe, J. B., Kriegeskorte, N., Thompson, R., & Calder, A. J. (2012). Direction-sensitive codes for observed head turns in human superior temporal sulcus. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 735-744.
Carpenter, M., Pennington, B.F., & Rogers, S.J. (2002). Interrelation among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 91-106.
Caruana, N., Stieglitz Ham, H., Brock, J., Woolgar, A., Kloth, N., Palermo, R., & McArthur, G. (2018). Joint attention difficulties in autistic adults: an interactive eye-tracking study. Autism, 22(4), 502-512.
Castiello, U. (2003). Understanding other people’s actions: Intention and attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 416–430.
Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.
Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Automatic attention cueing through eye movement in 2‐year‐old children with autism. Child development, 74(4), 1108-1122.
Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2012). Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(8), 903-913.
Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biological psychiatry, 74(3), 195-203.
Chawarska, K., Volkmar, F., & Klin, A. (2010). Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry,67(2), 178.
Chawarska, K., Ye, S., Shic, F., & Chen, L. (2016). Multilevel differences in spontaneous social attention in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Child development, 87(2), 543-557.
Chang, C. L., Yen, C. F., & Yang, P. (2013). Adaptive behaviors in high-functioning Taiwanese children with autism spectrum disorders: An investigation of the mediating roles of symptom severity and cognitive ability. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 43(6), 1347-1355.
Chetcuti, L., Hudry, K., Grant, M., & Vivanti, G. (2019). Object-directed imitation in autism spectrum disorder is differentially influenced by motoric task complexity, but not social contextual cues. Autism, 23(1), 199-211.
Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016a). Attention allocation in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3(3), 209-223.
Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016b). Social attention in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Research in developmental disabilities, 48, 79-93.
Colombo, J., & Salley, B. (2015). Biopsychosocial perspectives on the development of attention in infancy. In S. D. Calkins (Ed.), Handbook of infant biopsychosocial development (pp. 71-96). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 28(6), 479-485.
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental psychology, 40(2), 271.
Dupierrix, E., de Boisferon, A. H., Méary, D., Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., Di Giorgio, E., ... & Pascalis, O. (2014). Preference for human eyes in human infants. Journal of experimental child psychology, 123, 138-146.
Ebitz, R. B., & Platt, M. L. (2014). An evolutionary perspective on the behavioral consequences of exogenous oxytocin application. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 1-12.
Esseily, R., & Fagard, J. (2013). Ostensive cues orient 10-month-olds’ attention toward the task but delay learning. Psychology, 4(07), 20.
Engel, A. K., Maye, A., Kurthen, M., & König, P. (2013). Where's the action? The pragmatic turn in cognitive science. Trends in cognitive sciences, 17(5), 202-209.
Ewing, L., Pellicano, E., & Rhodes, G. (2013). Reevaluating the selectivity of face-processing difficulties in children and adolescents with autism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 342-355.
Falck‐Ytter, T., Fernell, E., Gillberg, C., & Von Hofsten, C. (2010). Face scanning distinguishes social from communication impairments in autism. Developmental science, 13(6), 864-875.
Falck-Ytter, T., Fernell, E., Hedvall, Å. L., von Hofsten, C., & Gillberg, C. (2012). Gaze performance in children with autism spectrum disorder when observing communicative actions. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 42(10), 2236-2245.
Falck-Ytter, T., Thorup, E., & Bölte, S. (2015). Brief report: lack of processing bias for the objects other people attend to in 3-year-olds with autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 45(6), 1897-1904.
Falck-Ytter, T., & von Hofsten, C. (2011). How special is social looking in ASD: A review. Progress in brain research, 189, 209-222.
Fischer, J., Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., & Kanwisher, N. (2014). Unimpaired attentional disengagement and social orienting in children with autism. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 214-223.
Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S. R., Findlay, J. M., & Stanton, E. C. (2008). Brief report: Young adults with autism spectrum disorder show normal attention to eye-gaze information-evidence from a new change blindness paradigm. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38,1785-1790.
Franchini, M., Armstrong, V. L., Schaer, M., & Smith, I. M. (2018). Initiation of joint attention and related visual attention processes in infants with autism spectrum disorder: Literature review. Child Neuropsychology, 1-31.
Frank, M. C., Vul, E., & Saxe, R. (2011). Measuring the development of social attention using free‐viewing. Infancy, 17(4), 355-375.
Frazier, T. W., Strauss, M., Klingemier, E. W., Zetzer, E. E., Hardan, A. Y., Eng, C., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2017). A meta-analysis of gaze differences to social and nonsocial information between individuals with and without autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(7), 546-555.
Freeth, M., & Bugembe, P. (2019). Social partner gaze direction and conversational phase; factors affecting social attention during face-to-face conversations in autistic adults?. Autism, 23(2), 503-513.
Freeth, M., Chapman, P., Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2010). Do gaze cues in complex scenes capture and direct the attention of high functioning adolescents with ASD? Evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 40(5), 534-547.
Freeth, M., Ropar, D., Mitchell, P., Chapman, P., & Loher, S. (2011). Brief report: How adolescents with ASD process social information in complex scenes. Combining evidence from eye movements and verbal descriptions. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 41(3), 364-371.
Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological bulletin, 133(4), 694-724.
Frith, U. (2012). Why we need cognitive explanations of autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(11), 2073-2092.
Gliga, T., Elsabbagh, M., Hudry, K., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., & BASIS Team. (2012). Gaze following, gaze reading, and word learning in children at risk for autism. Child development, 83(3), 926-938.
Gonsiorowski, A., Williamson, R. A., & Robins, D. L. (2016). Brief report: Imitation of object-directed acts in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 46(2), 691-697.
Gotham, K., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2009). Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 39(5), 693-705.
Gotham, K., Risi, S., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2007). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 37(4), 613.
Gowen, E., Vabalas, A., Casson, A. J., & Poliakoff, E. (2020). Instructions to attend to an observed action increase imitation in autistic adults. Autism, 24(3), 730-743.
Guillon, Q., Hadjikhani, N., Baduel, S., & Rogé, B. (2014). Visual social attention in autism spectrum disorder: Insights from eye tracking studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 279-297.
Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., & Moore, M. J. (1977). Eye contact and face scanning in early infancy. Science, 198(4319), 853-855.
Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2009). Research review: Goals, intentions and mental states: Challenges for theories of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(8), 881-892.
Hanley, M., McPhillips, M., Mulhern, G., & Riby, D. M. (2013). Spontaneous attention to faces in Asperger syndrome using ecologically valid static stimuli. Autism, 17(6), 754-761.
Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology review, 20(3), 290-322.
Harrison, P. L., & Oakland, T. (2003). Adaptive Behavior Assessment System—Second Edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Hayward, D. A., & Ristic, J. (2013). The uniqueness of social attention revisited: Working memory load interferes with endogenous but not social orienting. Experimental Brain Research, 231, 405-414.
Heron-Delaney, M., Wirth, S., & Pascalis, O. (2011). Infants' knowledge of their own species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 366(1571), 1753-1763.
Hill, T. L., Gray, S. A., Kamps, J. L., & Varela, R. E. (2015). Age and adaptive functioning in children and adolescents with ASD: The effects of intellectual functioning and ASD symptom severity. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 45(12), 4074-4083.
Hobson, J. A., & Hobson, R. P. (2007). Identification: The missing link between joint attention and imitation?. Development and Psychopathology, 19(2), 411-431.
Hus, V., Gotham, K., & Lord, C. (2014). Standardizing ADOS domain scores: Separating severity of social affect and restricted and repetitive behaviors. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 44(10), 2400-2412.
Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of general psychiatry, 65(8), 946-954.
Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2009). Heterogeneity and homogeneity across the autism spectrum: the role of development. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 471-473.
Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427.
Kaiser, M. D., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2012). Disrupted action perception in autism: behavioral evidence, neuroendophenotypes, and diagnostic utility. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 2(1), 25-35.
Kliemann, D., Dziobek, I., Hatri, A., Baudewig, J., & Heekeren, H. R. (2012). The role of the amygdala in atypical gaze on emotional faces in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(28), 9469-9476.
Klin, A., & Jones, W. (2008). Altered face scanning and impaired recognition of biological motion in a 15‐month‐old infant with autism. Developmental science, 11(1), 40-46.
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 345-360.
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of general psychiatry, 59(9), 809-816.
Klin, A., Shultz, S., & Jones, W. (2015). Social visual engagement in infants and toddlers with autism: early developmental transitions and a model of pathogenesis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 50, 189-203.
Koldewyn, K., Weigelt, S., Kanwisher, N., & Jiang, Y. (2013). Multiple object tracking in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 43(6), 1394-1405.
Kwon, M. K., Moore, A., Barnes, C. C., Cha, D., & Pierce, K. (2019). Typical levels of eye-region fixation in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder across multiple contexts. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 58(10), 1004-1015.
Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2013). Autism. The Lancet, 383 (9920), 896–910.
Leekam, S. (2016). Social cognitive impairment and autism: what are we trying to explain?. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 371(1686).
Leekam, S. R., & Ramsden, C. A. (2006). Dyadic orienting and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 36(2), 185.
Leekam, S., Tandos, J., McConachie, H., Meins, E., Parkinson, K., Wright, C., ... & Couteur, A. L. (2007). Repetitive behaviours in typically developing 2-year‐olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(11), 1131-1138.
Lewkowicz, D. J., & Hansen-Tift, A. M. (2012). Infants deploy selective attention to the mouth of a talking face when learning speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(5), 1431-1436.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-WPS (ADOS-WPS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Luyster, R. J., Powell, C., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2014). Neural measures of social attention across the first years of life: characterizing typical development and markers of autism risk. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 131-143.
Machado, C. J., Bliss-Moreau, E., Platt, M. L., & Amaral, D. G. (2011). Social and nonsocial content differentially modulates visual attention and autonomic arousal in Rhesus macaques. PloS one, 6(10), e26598.
Magiati, I., Tay, X. W., & Howlin, P. (2014). Cognitive, language, social and behavioural outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review of longitudinal follow-up studies in adulthood. Clinical psychology review, 34(1), 73-86.
Markodimitraki, M., Kypriotaki, M., Ampartzaki, M., & Manolitsis, G. (2013). Effects of context and facial expression on imitation tasks in preschool children with autism. Early Child Development and Care, 183(9), 1276-1292.
Maye, M., Gaston, D., Godina, I., Conrad, J. A., Rees, J., Rivera, R., & Lushin, V. (2020). Playful but mindful: How to best use positive affect in treating toddlers with autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(3), 336-338.
Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2004). Influence of IQ and age in childhood autism: Lack of support for DSM-IV Asperger's disorder. Journal of developmental and physical disabilities, 16(3), 257-272.
McPartland, J. C., Webb, S. J., Keehn, B., & Dawson, G. (2011). Patterns of visual attention to faces and objects in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 41(2), 148-157.
Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C. E., Yale, M., Neal, R., & Schwartz, H. K. (2000). Gaze following, temperament, and language development in 6-month-olds: A replication and extension. Infant Behavior and Development, 23(2), 231-236.
Mullen, E. (1995). The Mullen Scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention, social-emotional approach in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 63-82.
Mundy, P. (2018). A review of joint attention and social‐cognitive brain systems in typical development and autism spectrum disorder. European Journal of Neuroscience, 47(6), 497-514.
Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A. V., & Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child development, 78(3), 938-954.
Mundy, P., & Burnette, C. (2005). Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders: Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and behavior (p. 650–681). John Wiley & Sons Inc.
Mundy, P., Card, J., & Fox, N. (2000). EEG correlates of the development of infant joint attention skills. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 36(4), 325-338.
Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). Early social communication scales (ESCS). Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami.
Mundy, P., & Neal, A. R. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation: Autism (Vol. 23) (p. 139–168). Academic Press.
Mundy, P., Sullivan, L., & Mastergeorge, A. M. (2009). A parallel and distributed‐processing model of joint attention, social cognition and autism. Autism research, 2(1), 2-21.
Nakano, T., Tanaka, K., Endo, Y., Yamane, Y., Yamamoto, T., Nakano, Y., ... & Kitazawa, S. (2010). Atypical gaze patterns in children and adults with autism spectrum disorders dissociated from developmental changes in gaze behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1696), 2935-2943.
Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: social learning through the second year. Developmental psychology, 42(3), 555.
Nielsen, M., Simcock, G., & Jenkins, L. (2008). The effect of social engagement on 24‐month‐olds’ imitation from live and televised models. Developmental science, 11(5), 722-731.
Norbury, C. F., Brock, J., Cragg, L., Einav, S., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2009). Eye‐movement patterns are associated with communicative competence in autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(7), 834-842.
Óturai, G., Kolling, T., & Knopf, M. (2013). Relations between 18-month-olds’ gaze pattern and target action performance: A deferred imitation study with eye tracking. Infant Behavior and Development, 36(4), 736-748.
Papagiannopoulou, E. A., Chitty, K. M., Hermens, D. F., Hickie, I. B., & Lagopoulos, J. (2014). A systematic review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies in children with autism spectrum disorders. Social Neuroscience, 9(6), 610-632.
Parish-Morris, J., Chevallier, C., Tonge, N., Letzen, J., Pandey, J., & Schultz, R. T. (2013). Visual attention to dynamic faces and objects is linked to face processing skills: a combined study of children with autism and controls. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 185.
Pellicano, E. (2007). Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: Clues to developmental primacy. Developmental psychology, 43(4), 974.
Pellicano, E. (2011). Psychological models of autism: An overview. In I. Roth & P. Rezaie (Eds.), Researching the Autism Spectrum: Contemporary Perspectives (pp. 219-265). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Peltola, M. J., Yrttiaho, S., & Leppänen, J. M. (2018). Infants' attention bias to faces as an early marker of social development. Developmental science, 21(6), e12687.
Piaget, J. (1963). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: Norton. (Originally published, 1952; Original French version, 1936).
Pierno, A. C., Becchio, C., Wall, M. B., Smith, A. T., Turella, L., & Castiello, U. (2006). When gaze turns into grasp. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 2130–2137.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3-25.
Posner, M.I. & Cohen, Y. (1984) Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Ed.), Attention and Performance X (pp. 55-66). Hillsadale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Riby, D. M., Hancock, P. J., Jones, N., & Hanley, M. (2013). Spontaneous and cued gaze-following in autism and Williams syndrome. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders, 5(1), 13.
Rice, K., Anderson, L. C., Velnoskey, K., Thompson, J. C., & Redcay, E. (2016). Biological motion perception links diverse facets of theory of mind during middle childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 146, 238–246.
Rice, K., Moriuchi, J. M., Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2012). Parsing heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: visual scanning of dynamic social scenes in school-aged children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(3), 238-248.
Robins, D. L., Casagrande, K., Barton, M., Chen, C. M. A., Dumont Mathieu, T., & Fein, D. (2014). Validation of the modified checklist for autism in toddlers, revised with follow-up (M-CHAT-R/F). Pediatrics, 133(1), 37–45.
Rogers, S. J., & Dawson, G. (2010). Early Start Denver Model for young children with autism: promoting language, learning, and engagement. New York: Guilford Press.
Rommelse, N. N., Van der Stigchel, S., & Sergeant, J. A. (2008). A review on eye movement studies in childhood and adolescent psychiatry. Brain and cognition, 68(3), 391-414.
Pons, F., Bosch, L., & Lewkowicz, D. J. (2015). Bilingualism modulates infants’ selective attention to the mouth of a talking face. Psychological science, 26(4), 490-498.
Sacrey, L. A. R., Armstrong, V. L., Bryson, S. E., & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2014). Impairments to visual disengagement in autism spectrum disorder: a review of experimental studies from infancy to adulthood. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 559-577.
Salley, B., & Colombo, J. (2016). Conceptualizing social attention in developmental research. Social Development, 25(4), 687-703.
Sasson, N. J., & Touchstone, E. W. (2014). Visual attention to competing social and object images by preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 44(3), 584-592.
Schultz, R. T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2-3), 125-141.
Shic, F., Macari, S., & Chawarska, K. (2014). Speech disturbs face scanning in 6-month-old infants who develop autism spectrum disorder. Biological psychiatry, 75(3), 231-237.
Shic, F., Wang, Q., Macari, S. L., & Chawarska, K. (2020). The role of limited salience of speech in selective attention to faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(4), 459-469.
Sigman, M., Dissanayake, C., Corona, R., & Espinosa, M. (2003). Social and cardiac responses of young children with autism. Autism, 7, 205-216.
Stagg, S. D., Linnell, K. J., & Heaton, P. (2014). Investigating eye movement patterns, language, and social ability in children with autism spectrum disorder. Development and psychopathology, 26(2), 529-537.
Swanson, M. R., & Siller, M. (2013). Patterns of gaze behavior during an eye-tracking measure of joint attention in typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(9), 1087-1096.
Tanaka, J. W., & Sung, A. (2016). The “eye avoidance” hypothesis of autism face processing. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 46(5), 1538-1552.
Tenenbaum, E. J., Amso, D., Righi, G., & Sheinkopf, S. J. (2017). Attempting to “increase intake from the input”: Attention and word learning in children with autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 47(6), 1791-1805.
Tenenbaum, E. J., Shah, R. J., Sobel, D. M., Malle, B. F., & Morgan, J. L. (2013). Increased focus on the mouth among infants in the first year of life: A longitudinal eye-tracking study. Infancy, 18(4), 534-553.
Tenenbaum, E. J., Sobel, D. M., Sheinkopf, S. J., Malle, B. F., & Morgan, J. L. (2015). Attention to the mouth and gaze following in infancy predict language development. Journal of Child Language, 42(6), 1173-1190.
Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & psychophysics, 51(6), 599-606.
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C., Dunham, P. J., & Dunham, P. (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103-130). Psychology Press.
Thorup, E., Nyström, P., Gredebäck, G., Bölte, S., & Falck-Ytter, T. (2016). Altered gaze following during live interaction in infants at risk for autism: An eye tracking study. Molecular autism, 7(1), 1-10.
Thorup, E., Nyström, P., Gredebäck, G., Bölte, S., Falck-Ytter, T., & EASE Team. (2018). Reduced alternating gaze during social interaction in infancy is associated with elevated symptoms of autism in toddlerhood. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 1-15.
Toth, K., Munson, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Dawson, G. (2006). Early predictors of communication development in young children with autism spectrum disorder: Joint attention, imitation, and toy play. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 36(8), 993-1005.
Tureck, K., Matson, J. L., Cervantes, P., & Konst, M. J. (2014). An examination of the relationship between autism spectrum disorder, intellectual functioning, and comorbid symptoms in children. Research in developmental disabilities, 35(7), 1766-1772.
Vabalas, A., Gowen, E., Poliakoff, E., & Casson, A. J. (2020). Applying machine learning to kinematic and eye movement features of a movement imitation task to predict autism diagnosis. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1-13.
Vanvuchelen, M., Van Schuerbeeck, L., Roeyers, H., & De Weerdt, W. (2013). Understanding the mechanisms behind deficits in imitation: Do individuals with autism know ‘what’to imitate and do they know ‘how’to imitate?. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(1), 538-545.
Venker, C. E., & Kover, S. T. (2015). An open conversation on using eye-gaze methods in studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(6), 1719-1732.
Vivanti, G., & Dissanayake, C. (2014). Propensity to imitate in autism is not modulated by the model's gaze direction: An eye‐tracking study. Autism Research, 7(3), 392-399.
Vivanti, G., Fanning, P. A., Hocking, D. R., Sievers, S., & Dissanayake, C. (2017). Social attention, joint attention and sustained attention in autism Spectrum disorder and Williams syndrome: Convergences and divergences. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 47(6), 1866-1877.
Vivanti, G., Hocking, D. R., Fanning, P., & Dissanayake, C. (2016a). Social affiliation motives modulate spontaneous learning in Williams syndrome but not in autism. Molecular Autism, 7(1), 40.
Vivanti, G., Hocking, D. R., Fanning, P., & Dissanayake, C. (2016b). Verbal labels increase the salience of novel objects for preschoolers with typical development and Williams syndrome, but not in autism. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders, 8(1), 46.
Vivanti, G., Nadig, A., Ozonoff, S., & Rogers, S. J. (2008). What do children with autism attend to during imitation tasks?. Journal of experimental child psychology, 101(3), 186-205.
Vivanti, G., Trembath, D., & Dissanayake, C. (2014). Mechanisms of imitation impairment in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(8), 1395-1405.
von Hofsten, C., Uhlig, H., Adell, M., & Kochukhova, O. (2009). How children with autism look at events. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(2), 556-569.
Wang, Q., Hoi, S. P., Wang, Y., Lam, C. M., Fang, F., & Yi, L. (2020). Gaze response to others’ gaze following in children with and without autism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(3), 320–329.
Wang, Q., Wall, C. A., Barney, E. C., Bradshaw, J. L., Macari, S. L., Chawarska, K., & Shic, F. (2020). Promoting social attention in 3‐year‐olds with ASD through gaze‐contingent eye tracking. Autism Research, 13(1), 61-73.
Wan, G., Kong, X., Sun, B., Yu, S., Tu, Y., Park, J., ... & Lin, Y. (2019). Applying eye tracking to identify autism spectrum disorder in children. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 49(1), 209-215.
Wass, S. V., Forssman, L., & Leppänen, J. (2014). Robustness and precision: How data quality may influence key dependent variables in infant eye‐tracker analyses. Infancy, 19(5), 427-460.
Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., & Gowen, E. (2012). Goal-directed and goal-less imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 42(8), 1739-1749.
Youden, W. J. (1950). Index for rating diagnostic tests. Cancer, 3(1), 32-35.
Young, G. S., Merin, N., Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: Predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Developmental science, 12(5), 798-814.
 
 
 
 
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
* *